Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-68945f75b7-qvshk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-05T19:41:29.139Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Training behavior by imitation: from parrots to people … to robots?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 December 2009

Irene M. Pepperberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, MIT School of Architecture and Planning and Brandeis University, USA
Diane V. Sherman
Affiliation:
New-Found Therapies, Inc., USA
Chrystopher L. Nehaniv
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Kerstin Dautenhahn
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Learning by imitation is not a unitary task; thus understanding its various levels in order to potentiate imitative learning is daunting, whether subjects are human, animal or inanimate. For some syndromes arising in childhood (e.g. autism), lack of imitative ability (i.e. excluding rote, meaningless, often involuntary mimetic behavior, e.g. echolalia) is a defining characteristic (Hobson and Lee, 1999; Williams et al., 2001). What constitutes imitation in animals is still under study (Hurley and Chater, 2005; Nehaniv and Dautenhahn, this volume). And computers (and, by inference, robots using computer intelligence) may be “smart” in terms of brute processing power, but their learning is limited to what is easily programmed. Most computers and robots are presently analogous to living systems trained in relatively simple conditioned stimulus-response paradigms: given specific input parameters (that can, of course, be numerous and diverse), computers quickly and efficiently produce a predetermined, correct output (Pepperberg, 2001); computers can, however, generally solve only those new problems that are similar to ones they have already been programmed to solve. And although connectionist models are making significant advances, allowing generalization beyond training sets for a number of individual problems (e.g. Schlesinger and Parisi, 2004), at present most computational mechanisms – and autistic children – cannot learn in ways normal humans do. No one model, for example, does all the following: form new abstract representations, manipulate these representations to attain concrete goals, transfer information acquired in one domain to manage tasks in another, integrate new and existing disparate forms of knowledge (e.g. linguistic, contextual, emotional) to solve novel problems – nor can any one model achieve these behavior patterns through imitation in ways managed by every normal young child (see Lieberman, 2001).

Type
Chapter
Information
Imitation and Social Learning in Robots, Humans and Animals
Behavioural, Social and Communicative Dimensions
, pp. 383 - 406
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M. A. (2002). The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 229–79.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A., Billard, A., Iacoboni, M. and Oztop, E. (2000). Synthetic brain imaging: grasping, mirror neurons and imitation. Neural Networks, 13, 975–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A. and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Neural expectations: a possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393–424.Google Scholar
Avikainen, S., Kulomaeki, T. and Hari, R. (1999). Normal movement reading in Asperger subjects. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 10, 3467–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldissera, F., Cavallari, P., Craighero, L. and Fadiga, L, . (2001). Modulation of spinal excitability during observation of hand actions in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(1), 190–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origin and Role in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 131–58.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y. and Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billard, A. (2002). Imitation: a means to enhance learning of a synthetic protolanguage in autonomous robots. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 281–310.Google Scholar
Billard, A. and Arbib, M. (2002). Mirror neurons and the neural basis for learning by imitation: computational modeling. In Stamenov, M. I. (ed.), Mirror Neurons and the Evolution of Brain and Language. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, 343–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (2000). The intentionality model: how to learn a word, any word. In Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bloom, L., Smith, L. B., Woodward, A. L., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M. and Hollich, G. (eds.), Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press, 124–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breazeal, C. and Scassellati, B. (2002). Challenges in building robots that imitate people. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 363–90.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: the Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Galese, V., Seitz, R J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G. and Freund, H.-J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neurosciences, 13, 400–4.Google ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. (2002). Imitation of novel complex actions: what does evidence from animals mean? In Slater, P. J. B., Rosenblatt, J. S., Snowdon, C. T. and Roper, T. J. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Vol. 31, 77–105.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of imitation in social learning. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 211–28.Google Scholar
Chaminade, T., Meary, D., Orliaguet, J.-P. and Decety, J. (2001). Is perceptual anticipation a motor simulation? A PET study. Brain Imaging, 12(17), 3669–74.Google ScholarPubMed
Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A. N. and Decety, J. (2002). Does the end justify the means? A PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation. NeuroImage, 15, 318–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlop, M. H., Schreibman, L. and Tryon, A. S. (1983). Learning through observation: the effects of peer modeling on acquisition and generalization in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11(3), 355–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens. San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Inc.Google Scholar
Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (2002). The agent-based perspective on imitation. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–40.Google Scholar
Demiris, J. and Hayes, G. (2002). Imitation as a dual-route process featuring predictive and learning components: a biologically plausible computational model. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 327–62.Google Scholar
Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G. and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 399–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G. and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogassi, L. (2000). Mirror neurons and language origin. Paper presented at the International Conference on the Development of Mind, Tokyo, Japan, August.Google Scholar
Gallese, V. and Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garfinkle, A. N. and Schwartz, I. S. (2002). Peer imitation: increasing social interactions in children with autism and other developmental disabilities in inclusive preschool classrooms. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 22(1), 26–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsburg, N. (1960). Conditioned vocalization in the budgerigar. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 53(2), 183–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsburg, N. (1963). Conditioned vocalization in the mynah bird. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(6), 1061–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, R. M. and Barker, J. A. (1994). Autism and the “theory of mind” debate. In Graham, G. and Stephens, G. L. (eds.), Philosophical Psychopathology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 163–81.Google Scholar
Gossette, R. L. (1969/1980). Personal communication to O. H. Mowrer. Psychology of Language and Learning. New York: Plenum Press, 105–6.Google Scholar
Gramza, A. F. (1970). Vocal mimicry in captive budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 27(8), 971–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosslight, J. H. and Zaynor, W. C. (1967). Vocal behavior of the mynah bird. In , K. Salzinger and Salzinger, S. (eds.), Research in Verbal Behavior and Some Neuro-physiological Implications. New York: Academic Press, 5–9.Google Scholar
Grosslight, J. H., Zaynor, W. C. and Lively, B. L. (1964). Speech as a stimulus for differential vocal behavior in the mynah bird (Gracula religiosa). Psychonomic Science, 11, 7–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P. and Hill, K. (1996). Can we teach children with autism to understand emotions, belief, or pretence?Development and Psychopathology, 8(2), 345–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, P. R. and Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(4), 649–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K. and Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 262, 1–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurley, S. and Chater, N. (eds.) (2005). Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ihrig, K. and Wolchik, S. A. (1988). Peer versus adult models and autistic children's learning: acquisition, generalization, and maintenance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(1), 67–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaervelaeinen, J., Schürmann, M., Avikainen, S., Hari, R. (2001). Stronger reactivity of the human primary motor cortex during observation of live rather than video motor acts. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 12(16), 3493–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, E D. (2004). Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016, 749–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarvis, E., Güntürkün, O., Bruce, L., Csillag, A., Karten, H., Kuenzel, W., Medina, L., Paxinos, G., Perkel, D. J., Shimizu, T., Striedter, G., Wild, J. M., Ball, G. F., Dugas-Ford, J., Durand, S. E., Hough, G. E., Husband, S., Kubikova, L., Lee, D. W., Mello, C. V., Powers, A., Siang, C., Smulders, T. V., Wada, K., White, S. A., Yamamoto, K., Yu, J., Reiner, A. and Butler, A. B. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate evolution. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 151–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, E. D. and Mello, C. V. (2000). Molecular mapping of brain areas involved in parrot vocal communication. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 419, 1–31.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Wicker, B. and Perrett, D. I. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain and Cognition, 44, 280–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, J. E. (1966). Transfer relationships between learning set and concept formation in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 61(3), 414–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koegel, R. L. and Rincover, A. (1974). Treatment of psychotic children in a classroom environment: I. Learning in a large group. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7(1), 45–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanquetot, R. (1989). The effectiveness of peer modeling with autistic children. Journal of the Multi-handicapped Person, 2(1), 25–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1971). Of language, knowledge, apes, and brains. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1(1), 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. H. (1973). Biological aspects of language. In Miller, G. A. (ed.), Communication, Language, and Meaning. New York: Basic Books, 49–60.Google Scholar
Lieberman, H. (ed.) (2001). Your Wish Is My Command: Programming by Example. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann/Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lovibond, P. F. and Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28(1), 3–26.Google ScholarPubMed
Marler, P. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mebes, H.-D. (1978). Pair-specific duetting in the Peach-faced Lovebird. Naturwissenschaften, 65, 66–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michlmayr, M. (2002). Simulation theory versus theory theory: theories concerning the ability to read minds. Diplomarbeit zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Magisters an der Geisteswissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Leopold-Franzens-Universität Innsbruck.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1952). The autism theory of speech development and some clinical applications. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 17, 263–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. H. (1954). A psychologist looks at language. American Psychologist, 9, 660–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1969/1980). Theory and Research – A Review. Urbana: University of Illinois. Mimeographed, cited in O. Hobart Mowrer, Psychology of Language and Learning. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1985). Social modeling theory: a possible framework for understanding avian vocal learning. Auk, 102(4), 854–64.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1990). Cognition in an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): further evidence for comprehension of categories and labels. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104(1), 41–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1994). Vocal learning in African Grey parrots: effects of social interaction. Auk, 111(2), 300–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex Studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2001). Lessons from cognitive ethology: animals models for ethological computing. Proceedings of Epigenetic Robots, Lund, Sweden, September, 5–12.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2002) Allospecific referential speech acquisition in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): evidence for multiple levels of avian vocal imitation. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 109–31.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2004). Evolution of communication from an avian perspective. In Oller, K. and Grieble, U. (eds.), Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 171–92.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (2005). Insights into vocal imitation in Grey Parrots (Psittacus erithacus). In Hurley, S. L., Chater, N. (eds.), Perspectives on Imitation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Vol. 1, 243–62.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and Brezinsky, M. V. (1991). Relational learning by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): discriminations based on relative size. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105(3), 286–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and Lynn, S. K. (2000). Possible levels of animal consciousness with reference to Grey parrots. American Zoologist, 40(6), 893–901.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and McLaughlin, M. A. (1996). Effect of avian–human joint attention on vocal learning by Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(3), 286–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., Gardiner, L. I. and Luttrell, L. J. (1999). Limited contextual vocal learning in the Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): the effect of co-viewers on videotaped instruction. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(2), 158–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., Naughton, J. R. and Banta, P. A. (1998). Allospecific vocal learning by grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus): A failure of videotaped instruction under certain conditions. Behavioural Processes, 42(2–3), 139–158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pepperberg, I. M., Sandefer, R. M., Noel, D. A. and Ellsworth, C. P. (2000). Vocal learning in the Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus): effect of species identity and number of trainers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(4), 371–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and Sherman, D. V. (2000). Proposed use of two-part interactive modeling as a means to increase functional skills in children with a variety of disabilities. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 12(4), 213–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pepperberg, I. M. and Sherman, D. V. (2002). Use of two-part interactive modeling as a potential means to engender social behavior in children with various disabilities. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, 138–53.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. and Sherman, D. V. (2004). Use of two-part interactive modeling as a potential means to engender empathy in children within the autistic spectrum. IMFAR conference, Sacramento, CA.
Pepperberg, I. M. and Shive, H. R. (2001). Simultaneous development of vocal and physical object combinations by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): bottle caps, lids, and labels. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(4), 376–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierce, K. and Schreibman, L. (1995). Increasing complex social behaviors in children with autism: effects of peer-implemented pivotal response training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(3), 285–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierce, K. and Schreibman, L. (1997a). Multiple peer use of pivotal response training social behaviors of classmates with autism: results from trained and untrained peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(1), 157–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, K. and Schreibman, L. (1997b). Using peer trainers to promote social behavior in autism: are they effective at enhancing multiple social modalities?Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 12(4), 207–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollard, I. (2001). A comparison of the model/rival approach and a standard approach for teaching children with autism to understand belief, emotion and pretence. MA thesis, Department of Psychology, University of New England, Armidale, New South Wales, Australia.
Premack, D. (1978). On the abstractness of human concepts: why it would be difficult to talk to a pigeon. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honig, W. K. (eds.), Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 421–51.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1983). The codes of man and beasts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 125–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of actions. Nature Review Neurology, 2, 661–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruby, P. and Decety, J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nature Neuroscience, 4(5), 546–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumbaugh, D. M. and Pate, J. L. (1984). Primates learning by levels. In Greenberg, G. and Tobach, E. (eds.), Behavioral Evolution and Integrative Levels. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum, 221–40.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. Murphy, , J. Sevcik, , R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L. and Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 233, 1–258.Google Scholar
Schlesinger, M. and Parisi, D. (2004). Beyond backprop: emerging trends in connectionist models of development: an introduction. Developmental Science, 7, 131–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherratt, D. (2002). Developing pretend play in children with autism: a case study. Autism, 6(2), 169–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, I. M. and Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strain, P. S., Kerr, M. M. and Ragland, E. U. (1979). Effects of peer-mediated social initiations and prompting/reinforcement procedures on the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9(1), 41–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Striedter, G. (1994). The vocal control pathways in budgerigars differ from those in songbirds. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 343, 35–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: connections in autism. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 124–49.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. N. (1974). Animal and Human Nature.New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. N. and North, M. E. W. (1965). Origin and significance of the power of vocal imitation: with special reference to the antiphonal singing of birds. Nature, 208, 19–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todt, D. (1975). Social learning of vocal patterns and modes of their applications in Grey parrots. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 39, 178–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D. M. (2002). “Do monkeys ape?” Ten years after. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 471–99.Google Scholar
Wessberg, J., Stambaugh, C. R., Kralik, J. D., Beck, P. D., Laubach, M., Chapin, J. K., Kim, J., Biggs, S. J., Srinivasan, M. A. and Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2000). Real-time prediction of hand trajectory by ensembles of cortical neurons in primates. Nature, 408, 361–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. (2002). Imitation of sequential and hierarchical structure in action: experimental studies with children and chimpanzees. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 191–210.Google Scholar
Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T. and Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, N. S., Gales, M. E., Shane, E. and Shane, M. (2001). The developmental trajectory from amodal perception to empathy and communication: the role of mirror neurons in this process. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 21(1), 94–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodard, W. T. and Bitterman, M. E. (1972). Further studies of reversal learning with singly presented stimuli in pigeons and goldfish. Psychonomic Science, 28, 170–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×