Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-cnmwb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T04:14:18.375Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Influences of Odors on Mood and Affective Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 September 2009

Rachel S. Herz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Box 1853, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
Catherine Rouby
Affiliation:
Université Lyon I
Benoist Schaal
Affiliation:
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Paris
Danièle Dubois
Affiliation:
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Paris
Rémi Gervais
Affiliation:
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Paris
A. Holley
Affiliation:
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Paris
Get access

Summary

In this chapter we shall review a wide range of research that shows how odors can influence mood, cognition, and behavior. This review, though not exhaustive, offers a comprehensive overview of the field. As background for this analysis, a discussion of odor-associative learning will first be given. The topics to be covered will then include the effects of odor exposure on (1) mood and specific emotions, (2) attitudes, work efficiency, and perceived health, (3) emotional memory, and (4) emotionally conditioned behavior. In addition to the behavioral evidence, neuroanatomic substantiation for the special relationship between odor and emotional associations will be presented.

Odor-associative Learning

The aim of the following paragraphs is to illustrate that almost all our responses to odors are learned, rather than innate. Evidence to support this idea comes primarily from research with infants and children. Although some work suggests that young children show adult-like preferences for certain odors (Schmidt and Beauchamp, 1988), most research with this age group indicates that children often do not differentiate between odors that adults find either very unpleasant or pleasant, such as butyric acid (rancid butter) versus amyl acetate (banana) (Engen, 1988; Schaal, Soussignan, and Marlier, Chapter 26, this volume), or they may have responses opposite to adult preferences, such as liking the smell of synthetic sweat and feces (Stein, Ottenberg, and Roulet, 1958; Engen, 1982). By age eight, however, most children's hedonic responses to odors mimic those of adults. Odor learning begins prior to birth.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aggleton J P & Young A E (2000). The Enigma of the Amygdala: On Its Contribution to Human Emotion. In: Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion, ed. R D Lane & L Nadel, pp. 106–28. Oxford University Press
, Baeyens F, , Wrzesniewski A, , Houwer J, & , Eelen P (1996). Toilet Rooms, Body Massages, and Smells: Two Field Studies on Human Evaluative Odor Conditioning. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social 15:77–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Balogh R D & , Porter R H (1986). Olfactory Preference Resulting from Mere Exposure in Human Neonates. Infant Behavior and Development 9:395–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Baron R A (1983). “Sweet Smell of Success”? The Impact of Pleasant Artificial Scents on Evaluations of Job Applicants. Journal of Applied Psychology 68:709–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Baron R A (1990). Environmentally-induced Positive Affect: Its Impact on Self-efficacy, Task Performance, Negotiation, and Conflict. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 20:368–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Baron R A (1997). The Sweet Smell of Helping: Effects of Pleasant Ambient Fragrance on Prosocial Behavior in Shopping Malls. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23:489–503Google Scholar
, Baron R A & , Bell P A (1976). Aggression and Heat: The Influence of Ambient Temperature, Negative Affect, and a Cooling Drink on Physical Aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33:245–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Baron R A & , Bronfen M I (1994). A Whiff of Reality: Empirical Evidence Concerning the Effects of Pleasant Fragrances on Work-related Behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 24:1179–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Baron R A, , Rea M S, & , Daniels S G (1993). Effects of Indoor Lighting on the Performance of Cognitive Tasks and Interpersonal Behaviors: The Potential Mediating Role of Positive Affect. Motivation and Emotion 16:1–33Google Scholar
, Baron R A & , Thomley J (1994). A Whiff of Reality: Positive Affect as a Potential Mediator of the Effects of Pleasant Fragrances on Task Performance and Helping. Environment and Behavior 26:766–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Cahill L, , Babinsky R, , Markowilsch H J, & , McGaugh J (1995). Amygdala and Emotional Memory. Nature 377:295–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain W S (1987). Indoor Air as a Source of Annoyance. In: Environmental Annoyance: Characterization, Measurement and Control, ed. H S Koelega, pp. 189–200. Amsterdam: Elsevier
, Cain W S & , Johnson F Jr (1978). Lability of Odor Pleasantness: Influence of Mere Exposure. Perception 7:459–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Chen D & , Haviland-Jones J (1999). Rapid Mood Change and Human Odors. Physiology and Behavior 68:241–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Chu S & , Downes J J (2000). Odour-evoked Autobiographical Memories: Psychological Investigations of Proustian Phenomena. Chemical Senses 25:111–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark M S (1991). Mood and Helping: Mood as a Motivator of Helping and Helping as a Regulator of Mood. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Colligan M J & Murphy L (1982). A Review of Mass Psychogenic Illness in Work Settings. In: Mass Psychogenic Illness: A Social Psychological Analysis, ed. M Colligan, J Pennbaker, & L Murphy, pp. 35–56. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
, Dalton P (1996). Odor Perception and Beliefs about Risk. Chemical Senses 21:447–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Dalton P (1999). Cognitive Influences on Health Symptoms from Acute Chemical Exposure. Health Psychology 18:579–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Dalton P, , Wysocki C J, , Brody M J, & , Lawley H J (1997). The Influence of Cognitive Bias on the Perceived Odor, Irritation and Health Symptoms from Chemical Exposure. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health 69:407–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlichman H & Bastone L (1992). The Use of Odour in the Study of Emotion. In: Fragrance: The Psychology and Biology of Perfume, ed. S Van Toller & G H Dodd, pp. 143–59. London: Elsevier
, Ehrlichman H & , Halpern J N (1988). Affect and Memory: Effects of Pleasant and Unpleasant Odors on Retrieval of Happy and Unhappy Memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55:769–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Ehrlichman H, , Kuhl S B, , Zhu J, & , Warrenburg S (1997). Startle Reflex Modulation by Pleasant and Unpleasant Odors in a Between-Subjects Design. Psychophysiology 34:726–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Eich J E, & , Metcalf J (1989). Mood Dependent Memory for Internal versus External Events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 15:443–55Google Scholar
Eichenbaum H (1996). Olfactory Perception and Memory. In: The Mind–Brain Continuum, ed. R Llinas & P Churchland, pp. 173–202. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Engen T (1982). The Perception of Odors. New York: Academic Press
Engen T (1988). The Acquisition of Odour Hedonics. In: Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance, ed. S Van Toller & G H Dodd, pp. 79–90. London: Chapman & HallCrossRef
, Epple G & , Herz R S (1999). Ambient Odors Associated to Failure Influence Cognitive Performance in Children. Developmental Psychobiology 35:103–73.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Herz R S (1998a). An Examination of Objective and Subjective Measures of Experience Associated to Odors, Music and Paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts 16:137–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Herz R S (1998b). Are Odors the Best Cues to Memory? A Cross-Modal Comparison of Associative Memory Stimuli. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 855:670–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herz R S (2001). How Odor-evoked Memories Differ from Other Memory Experiences: Experimental Investigations into the Proustian Phenomenon. In: Compendium of Olfactory Research, ed. T Lorig, pp. 23–38. New York: Olfactory Research Fund
, Herz R S & , Cupchik G C (1992). An Experimental Characterization of Odor-evoked Memories in Humans. Chemical Senses 17:519–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Herz R S & , Cupchik G C (1995). The Emotional Distinctiveness of Odor-evoked Memories. Chemical Senses 20:517–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Herz R S & , Schooler J W (2002). A Naturalistic Study of Autobiographical Memories Evoked to Olfactory versus Visual Cues. American Journal of Psychology 115Google Scholar
, Hinton P B & , Henley T B (1993). Cognitive and Affective Components of Stimuli Presented in Three Modes. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31:595–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen A M (1984). Toward Understanding the Role of Affect in Cognition. In: Handbook of Social Cognition, ed. A L Wyer & T K Scrull, pp. 179–236. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
King J R (1988). Anxiety Reduction Using Fragrances. In: Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance, ed. S Van Toller & G H Dodd, pp. 121–44. London: Chapman & HallCrossRef
, Kirk-Smith M D, , Toller C, & , Dodd G H (1983). Unconscious Odour Conditioning in Human Subjects. Biological Psychology 17:221–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Kjaergaard S, , Pederson O F, & , Molhave L (1992). Sensitivity of the Eyes to Airborne Irritant Stimuli. Influence of Individual Characteristics. Archives of Environmental Health 47:45–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Kline N A & , Rausch J L (1985). Olfactory Precipitants of Flashbacks in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Case Reports. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 46:383–4Google Scholar
, Knasko S C (1992). Ambient Odor's Effect on Creativity, Mood, and Perceived Health. Chemical Senses 17:27–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Knasko S (1995). Pleasant Odors and Congruency: Effects on Approach Behavior. Chemical Senses 20:479–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Knasko S C, , Gilbert A N, & , Sabini J (1990). Emotional State, Physical Well-being and Performance in the Presence of Feigned Ambient Odor. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 20:1345–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Laird D A (1935). What Can You Do With Your Nose?Scientific Monthly 41:126–30Google Scholar
Lawless H (1991). Effects of Odors on Mood and Behavior: Aromatherapy and Related Effects. In: The Human Sense of Smell, ed. D G Laing, R L Doty, & W Breipohl, pp. 361–87. New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRef
Le Guérer A (1994). Scent. New York: Kodansha America
Levine J M & McBurney D H (1986). The Role of Olfaction in Social Perception and Behaviour. In: Physical Appearance, Stigma and Social Behaviour: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 3, ed. C P Herman, M P Zanna, & E T Higgins, pp. 179–217. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Livesey P J (1986). Learning and Emotion: A Biological Synthesis. Vol. 1: Evolutionary Processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
, Lorig T S & , Schwartz G E (1988). Brain and Odor: I. Alteration of Human EEG by Odor Administration. Psychobiology 16:281–4Google Scholar
, Ludvigson H W & , Rottman T R (1989). Effects of Ambient Odors of Lavender and Cloves on Cognition, Memory, Affect and Mood. Chemical Senses 14:525–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, McCaffrey R J, , Lorig T S, , Pendrey D L, , McCutcheon N B, & , Garrett J C (1993). Odor-induced EEG Changes in PTSD Vietnam Veterans. Journal of Traumatic Stress 6:213–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean P D (1969). A Triune Concept of the Brain and Behavior. New York: Bowker
, Marlier L, , Schaal B, & , Soussignan R (1998). Neonatal Responsiveness to the Odor of Amniotic and Lacteal Fluids: A Test of Chemosensory Continuity. Child Development 69:611–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Mennella J A & , Beauchamp G K (1991). The Transfer of Alcohol to Human Milk: Effects on Flavor and Infants and the Infant's Behavior. New England Journal of Medicine 325:981–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Mennella J A & , Beauchamp G K (1993). The Effects of Repeated Exposure to Garlic-flavored Milk on the Nursling's Behavior. Pediatric Research 34:805–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Mennella J A & , Beauchamp G K (1996). The Human Infant's Responses to Vanilla Flavors in Mother's Milk and Formula. Infant Behavior and Development 19:13–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Mennella J A, , Johnson A, & , Beauchamp G K (1995). Garlic Ingestion by Pregnant Women Alters the Odor of Amniotic Fluid. Chemical Senses 20:207–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Miltner W, , Matjak M, , Braun C, , Diekmann H, & , Brody S (1994). Emotional Qualities of Odors and Their Influence on the Startle Reflex in Humans. Psychophysiology 31:107–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moncrieff R W (1966). Odour Preferences. New York: Wiley
, Nakano Y, , Kikuchi A, , Matsui H, & , Hatayama T (1992). A Study of Fragrance Impressions, Evaluation and Categorization. Tohoku Psychologica Folia 51:83–90Google Scholar
, Neutra R, , Lipscomb J, , Satin K, & , Shusterman D (1991). Hypotheses to Explain the Higher Symptom Rates Observed Around Hazardous Waste Sites. Environmental Health Perspectives 94:31–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, O'Mahoney M (1978). Smell Illusions and Suggestion: Reports of Smells Contingent on Tones Played on Radio and Television. Chemical Senses and Flavor 3:183–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Robin O, , Alaoui-Ismaïli O, , Dittmar A, & , Vernet-Maury E (1998). Emotional Responses Evoked by Dental Odors: An Evaluation from Autonomic Parameters. Journal of Dental Research 77:1638–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls E T (1999). The Brain and Emotion. Oxford University Press
, Rotton J (1983). Affective and Cognitive Consequences of Malodorous Pollution. Applied Social Psychology 4:171–91Google Scholar
, Rubin D C, , Groth E, & , Goldsmith D J (1984). Olfactory Cueing of Autobiographical Memory. American Journal of Psychology 97:493–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schaal B, , Marlier L, & , Soussignan R (1998). Olfactory Function in the Human Fetus: Evidence from Selective Neonatal Responsiveness to the Odor of Amniotic Fluid. Behavioral Neuroscience 112:1438–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaal B & Rouby C (1990). Le développement des sens chimiques: Influences exogènes prénatales, conséquences postnatales. In: Progrès en Néonatologie, vol. 10, ed. J P Relier, pp. 182–201. Basel: Karger
, Schiffman S S, , Miller E A, , Suggs M S, & , Graham B G (1995a). The Effects of Environmental Odors Emanating from Commercial Swine Operations on the Mood of Nearby Residents. Brain Research Bulletin 37:369–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schiffman S S, , Sattley-Miller E A, , Suggs M S, & , Graham B G (1995b). The Effect of Pleasant Odors and Hormone Status on Mood of Women at Midlife. Brain Research Bulletin 36:19–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schiffman S S & , Sieber J M (1991). New Frontiers in Fragrance Use. Cosmetics and Toiletries 106:39–45Google Scholar
, Schiffman S S, , Suggs M S, & , Sattley-Miller E A (1995c). Effect of Pleasant Odors on Mood of Males at Midlife: Comparison of African-American and European-American men. Brain Research Bulletin 36:31–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schleidt M & , Genzel C (1990). The Significance of Mother's Perfume for Infants in the First Weeks of Their Life. Ethology and Sociobiology 11:145–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schmidt H J & , Beauchamp G K (1988). Adult-like Odor Preferences and Aversions in Three Year Old Children. Child Development 59:1136–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Schwerdtfeger W L, , Buhl E H, & , Gemroth P (1990). Disynaptic Olfactory Input to the Hippocampus Mediated by Stellate Cells in the Entorhinal Cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology 194:519–34Google Scholar
, Stahl S M & , Lebedun M (1996). Mystery Gas: An Analysis of Mass Hysteria. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 15:44–50Google Scholar
, Staubli U, , Ivy G, & , Lynch G (1984). Hippocampal Denervation Causes Rapid Forgetting of Olfactory Information in Rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 81:5885–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Stein M, , Ottenberg M D, & , Roulet N (1958). A Study of the Development of Olfactory Preferences. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry 80:264–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Sullivan R M, , Taborsky-Barba S, , Mendoza S, , Itano A, , Leon M, , Cotman W, , Payne T, & , Lott I (1991). Olfactory Classical Conditioning in Neonates. Pediatrics 87:511–17Google Scholar
, Turner B H, , Mishkin M, & , Knapp M (1980). Organization of Amygdalopetal Projections from Modality-specific Cortical Association Areas in the Monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology 191:515–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Underwood B, , Froming W J, & , Moore B S (1977). Mood, Attention, and Altruism. Developmental Psychology 13:541–2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Vrana S R, , Spence E L, & , Lang P J (1988). The Startle Probe Response: A New Measure of Emotion?Journal of Abnormal Psychology 97:487–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Warm J S, , Dember W N, & , Parasuraman R (1990). Effects of Fragrances on Vigilance, Performance and Stress. Perfumer and Flavorist 15:16–17Google Scholar
, Winberg J & , Porter R H (1998). Olfaction and Human Neonatal Behaviour: Clinical Implications. Acta Paediatrica 87:6–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Wysocki C J & , Beauchamp G K (1984). Ability to Smell Androstenone Is Genetically Determined. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 81:4899–902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Zatorre R J, , Jones-Gotman M, , Evans A C, & , Meyer E (1992). Functional Localization and Lateralization of Human Olfactory Cortex. Nature 360:339–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×