Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-14T03:53:02.490Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

More than language is needed to represent and combine different core knowledge components

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2024

Peter Krøjgaard*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and the Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark peter@psy.au.dk trines@psy.au.dk osman@psy.au.dk
Trine Sonne
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and the Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark peter@psy.au.dk trines@psy.au.dk osman@psy.au.dk
Osman S. Kingo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and the Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark peter@psy.au.dk trines@psy.au.dk osman@psy.au.dk
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

We question Spelke's key claim that the medium, in which contents from different core knowledge systems can be represented and combined, is language-based. Recalling an episodic memory, playing chess, and conducting mental rotation are tasks where core knowledge information is represented and combined. Although these tasks can be described by means of language, these tasks are not inherently language-based. Hence, language may be an important subset of an abstraction medium – not the medium as such.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, T. A., & Fortin, N. J. (2013). The evolution of episodic memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 1037910386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campitelli, G. (2017). Cognitive processes in chess. In Hambrick, D. Z., Campitelli, G., & Macnamara, B. N. (Eds.), The science of expertise: Behavioral, neural, and genetic approaches to complex skill (pp. 3146). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coates, P. (2013). Chess, imagination, and perceptual understanding. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 73, 211242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, C. B., Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2014). The functions of autobiographical memory: An integrative approach. Memory (Hove, England), 22, 559581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T. (2023). More than language: Mental time travel, mentalizing, executive attention, and the left-hemisphere interpreter in human origins. Psychological Review, 130, 15921611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milivojevic, B., Hamm, J. P., & Corballis, M. C. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of mental rotation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 945959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (2003). Return of the mental image: Are the really pictures in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 113118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenbaum, R. S., Köhler, S., Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R., Black, S. E., Gao, F., & Tulving, E. (2005). The case of K.C.: Contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theory. Neuropsychologia, 43, 9891021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic systems approach to autobiographical memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 7983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 277311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saariluoma, P. (1991). Aspects of skilled imagery in blindfold chess. Acta Psychologica, 77, 6589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepard, R. N., & Mentzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science (New York, N.Y.), 171, 701703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S. (2022). What babies know: Core knowledge and composition (Vol. 1). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In Terrace, H. S. and Metcalfe, J. (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp. 356). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar