Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-jwnkl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-14T04:31:25.007Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Where is the baby in core knowledge?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2024

Hyowon Gweon*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA gweon@stanford.edu; pgzhu@stanford.edu https://sll.stanford.edu
Peter Zhu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA gweon@stanford.edu; pgzhu@stanford.edu https://sll.stanford.edu
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

What we know about what babies know – as represented by the core knowledge proposal – is perhaps missing a place for the baby itself. By studying the baby as an actor rather than an observer, we can better understand the origins of human intelligence as an interface between perception and action, and how humans think and learn about themselves in a complex world.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolph, K. E., & Hoch, J. E. (2019). Motor development: Embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 141164. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asaba, M., & Gweon, H. (2022). Young children infer and manage what others think about them. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 119(32), e2105642119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2105642119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigelow, A. E., & Rochat, P. (2006). Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother–infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy, 9(3), 313325. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chuey, A., Jara-Ettinger, J., & Gweon, H. (2023). Violation of epistemic expectations: Children monitor what others know and recognize unexpected sources of knowledge. In M. Goldwater, F. K. Anggoro, B. K. Hayes, & D. C. Ong (Eds.), Proceedings of the 45th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2751–2758). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Danjo, T., Toyoizumi, T., & Fujisawa, S. (2018). Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus. Science, 359(6372), 213218. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aao3898CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denison, S., & Xu, F. (2010). Twelve- to 14-month-old infants can predict single-event probability with large set sizes: Twelve- to 14-month-olds can predict single-event probability. Developmental Science, 13(5), 798803. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00943.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2017). From needs to goals and representations: Foundations for a unified theory of motivation, personality, and development. Psychological Review, 124(6), 689719. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenson, L., & Carey, S. (2003). Tracking individuals via object-files: Evidence from infants’ manual search. Developmental Science, 6(5), 568584. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G. (2002). The development of understanding self and agency. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 2646). Blackwell Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (Vol. 60, pp. 101136). Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M., & Kouider, S. (2016). Infants ask for help when they know they don't know. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(13), 34923496. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1515129113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gweon, H., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). 16-month-olds rationally infer causes of failed actions. Science, 332(6037), 1524. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gweon, H., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Schulz, L. E. (2010). Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(20), 90669071. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003095107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. S. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature, 370(6484), 5759. https://doi.org/10.1038/370057a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyman, G. D., Compton, A. M., Amemiya, J., Ahn, S., & Shao, S. (2021). Children's reputation management: Learning to identify what is socially valued and acting upon it. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(4), 315320. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211009516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The consciousness of self. In James, W. (Ed.), The principles of psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 291401). Henry Holt and Co.Google Scholar
Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., & Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? Features that elicit gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233238. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90581-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, S., Brooks, N. B., & Spelke, E. S. (2019). Origins of the concepts cause, cost, and goal in prereaching infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 2, 201904410201904416. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904410116Google Scholar
Moser, E. I., Roudi, Y., Witter, M. P., Kentros, C., Bonhoeffer, T., & Moser, M.-B. (2014). Grid cells and cortical representation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(7), 466481. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3766CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164173. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 3559. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515088808572924CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omer, D. B., Maimon, S. R., Las, L., & Ulanovsky, N. (2018). Social place-cells in the bat hippocampus. Science, 359(6372), 218224. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aao3474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2000). Perceived self in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(3–4), 513530. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00055-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovee, C. K., & Rovee, D. T. (1969). Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8(1), 3339. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(69)90025-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skerry, A. E., Carey, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). First-person action experience reveals sensitivity to action efficiency in prereaching infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(46), 1872818733. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1312322110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96(1), B1B11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S. (2022). What babies know: Core knowledge and composition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science, 348(6230), 9194. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa3799CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Streri, A., & Spelke, E. S. (1988). Haptic perception of objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 20(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(88)90022-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, A. J., Saxe, R., & Spelke, E. S. (2022). Infants infer potential social partners by observing the interactions of their parent with unknown others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 119(32), e2121390119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2121390119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69(1), 134. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, P., Dweck, C., & Gweon, H. (2023). Young children's curiosity about what others think about the self. In M. Goldwater, F. K. Anggoro, B. K. Hayes, & D. C. Ong (Eds.), Proceedings of the 45th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 835–842). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science SocietyGoogle Scholar